Quite A Box of Tricks v1.8-SHOCK serial key or number

Quite A Box of Tricks v1.8-SHOCK serial key or number

Quite A Box of Tricks v1.8-SHOCK serial key or number

Quite A Box of Tricks v1.8-SHOCK serial key or number

PMC

Kent C. Berridge

1Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI USA

Morten L. Kringelbach

2Department of Psychiatry, Warneford Hospital, University of Oxford, Oxford, OX3 7JX, UK

3Centre for Functionally Integrative Neuroscience, University of Aarhus, Aarhus C, Denmark

1Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI USA
2Department of Psychiatry, Warneford Hospital, University of Oxford, Oxford, OX3 7JX, UK
3Centre for Functionally Integrative Neuroscience, University of Aarhus, Aarhus C, Denmark
The publisher's final edited version of this article is available at Neuron
See other articles in PMC that cite the published article.

Abstract

Pleasure is mediated by well-developed mesocorticolimbic circuitry, and serves adaptive functions. In affective disorders anhedonia (lack of pleasure) or dysphoria (negative affect) can result from breakdowns of that hedonic system. Human neuroimaging studies indicate that surprisingly similar circuitry is activated by quite diverse pleasures, suggesting a common neural currency shared by all. Wanting for rewards is generated by a large and distributed brain system. Liking, or pleasure itself, is generated by a smaller set of hedonic hotspots within limbic circuitry. Those hotspots also can be embedded in broader anatomical patterns of valence organization, such as in a keyboard pattern of nucleus accumbens generators for desire versus dread. In contrast, some of the best known textbook candidates for pleasure generators, including classic pleasure electrodes and the mesolimbic dopamine system, may not generate pleasure after all. These emerging insights into brain pleasure mechanisms may eventually facilitate better treatments for affective disorders.

Introduction

The English word hedonic comes originally from the ancient Greek for pleasure (ἡ ´¿½®; in Latin script: hédoné), in turn derived from the word for sweet (ἡ ´ÍÂ, or h&#x;dús). Today hedonic refers to sensory pleasures as well as many higher types of pleasure (e.g., cognitive, social, aesthetic, and moral).

A goal of affective neuroscience is to understand how brain mechanisms generate pleasures, and also displeasures, and eventually find more effective treatments for affective disorders (Anderson and Adolphs, ; Damasio and Carvalho, ; Haber and Knutson, ; Heller et al., ; Kringelbach and Berridge, ; Panksepp, ). Capacity for normal pleasure is essential to healthy psychological function or well-being. Conversely, affective disorders can induce either the pathological absence of pleasure reactions (as in clinical anhedonia), or the presence of excessive displeasure (dysphoric emotions such as pain, disgust, depression, anxiety, or fear).

But is a neuroscience of pleasure feasible? Doubts that pleasure might be scientifically understood have been expressed for over a century. Early doubts stemmed from behaviorist convictions that only objective behavioral-neural reactions were eligible for scientific study, and never subjective experiences (including the experience of pleasure). However, progress in the past 50 years proves that many complex psychological processes involving subjective experience can be successfully studied and related to underlying brain mechanisms. Still, some objections persist today. For example, LeDoux&#x;s recent recommendation that affective neuroscientists should focus only on behavioral affective reactions, rather than on subjective emotions, shares those earlier concerns (LeDoux, ).

In our view, a neuroscience of pleasure can be pursued as successfully as the neuroscience of perception, learning, cognition or other well-studied psychological functions. The crucial test of this proposition is: can affective neuroscience produce important new conclusions into how brain systems mediate hedonic impact? Evidence in support of this, we think, now exists in the form of recent findings. In this article we discuss some of these new findings, including 1) separation of reward liking, wanting, and learning mechanisms in mesocorticolimbic circuitry; 2) identification of overlap in neural circuitry underlying sensory pleasures and higher pleasures; 3) identification of particular sites in prefrontal limbic cortex that encode pleasure impact; 4) mapping of surprisingly localized causal hedonic hotspots that generate amplifications of pleasure reactions; 5) discovery that nucleus accumbens (NAc) hotspot and coldspot mechanisms are embedded in an anatomically-tuned keyboard organization of generators in nucleus accumbens that extends beyond reward liking and wanting to negative emotions of fear and disgust; and 6) identification of multiple neurochemical modes within NAc mechanisms that can retune keyboard generators into flipping between oppositely-valenced motivations of desire and dread.

A neuroscience of pleasure

In a sense, pleasure can be thought of as evolution&#x;s boldest trick, serving to motivate an individual to pursue rewards necessary for fitness, yet in modern environments of abundance also inducing maladaptive pursuits such as addictions. An important starting point for understanding the underlying circuitry is to recognize that rewards involve a composite of several psychological components: liking (core reactions to hedonic impact), wanting (motivation process of incentive salience), and learning (Pavlovian or instrumental associations and cognitive representations) (Berridge and Robinson, ). These component processes also have discriminable neural mechanisms. The three processes can occur together at any time during the reward-behavior cycle, though wanting processes tend to dominate the initial appetitive phase, while liking processes dominate the subsequent consummatory phase that may lead to satiety. Learning, on the other hand, happens throughout the cycle. A neuroscience of reward seeks to map these components onto necessary and sufficient brain networks (see Figure 1).

Causal hedonic hotspots and coldspots in the brain

A) Top shows positive hedonic orofacial expressions (&#x;liking&#x;) elicited by sucrose taste in rat, orangutan, and newborn human infant. Negative aversive (&#x;disgust&#x;) reactions are elicited by bitter taste. B) shows sagittal view of hedonic hotspots in rat brain containing nucleus accumbens, ventral pallidum, and prefrontal cortex. Hotspots (red) depict sites where opioid stimulation enhances &#x;liking&#x; reactions elicited by sucrose taste. Coldspots (blue) show sites where the same opioid stimulation oppositely suppresses &#x;liking&#x; reactions to sucrose. C) Nucleus accumbens blow-up of medial shell shows effects of opioid microinjections in NAc hotspot and coldspot. (red/orange dots in hotspot = % increases in &#x;liking&#x; reactions; blue dots in coldspot = 50% reductions in &#x;liking&#x; reactions to sucrose). Panels show separate hedonic effects of mu opioid, delta opioid and kappa opioid stimulation via microinjections in NAc shell on sweetness &#x;liking&#x; reactions. Bottom row shows effects of mu, delta or kappa agonist microinjections on establishment of a learned place preference (i.e., red/orange dots in hotspot) or place avoidance (blue dots). Surprisingly similar patterns of anterior hedonic hotspots and posterior suppressive coldspots are seen for all three major types of opioid receptor stimulation. Modified from (Castro and Berridge, ).

To study pleasure comprehensively, good human neuroimaging studies are needed to explore correlative encoding of pleasant experiences, and good animal studies are needed to explore causation of underlying hedonic reactions. This two-pronged approach exploits a fundamental duality in hedonic processes, related to the objective versus subjective faces of pleasure (Damasio and Carvalho, ; Kringelbach and Berridge, ; Schooler and Mauss, ; Winkielman et al., ). Pleasure is sometimes assumed to be a purely subjective feeling. But pleasure also has objective features in the form of measurable hedonic reactions, both neural and behavioral, to valenced events. In this review we denote objective hedonic reactions as &#x;liking&#x; reactions (with quotes) to distinguish them from the subjective experience of liking (in the ordinary sense, without quotes). Objective hedonic reactions can be measured in both human and animal neuroscience studies, which together allow some comparisons across species and can lead to a more complete causal picture of how brain systems mediate hedonic impact.

Evolutionary origins of brain systems for hedonic reactions

The ultimate explanation for why pleasure encompasses both objective and subjective levels of reaction likely lies in evolutionary history. Darwin () originally suggested that affective reactions were selected by evolution for their useful functions, which were adapted into emotional expressions (Darwin, ). Following Darwin&#x;s logic, modern affective neuroscience also posits brain mechanisms of emotional reactions to mediate evolved survival functions (LeDoux, ), with emotional core features that can form the basis for studies of emotion across phylogeny (p. ) (Anderson and Adolphs, ), which can be usefully exploited by objective studies.

The selection of hedonic reactions has required the evolution of mammalian brains to dedicate millions of developing neurons into mesocorticolimbic patterns of reward circuitry (Haber and Knutson, ). Such neural investment was subject to the same selection pressures that shaped evolution of any other function. Hedonic circuitry was therefore unlikely to have been shaped into its present form, or to have persisted throughout evolution, unless objective affective reactions actually conveyed significant consequences in terms of benefits for survival and fitness (Anderson and Adolphs, ; Damasio, ; Kringelbach and Berridge, ; LeDoux, ; Panksepp, ). Objective affective reactions likely appeared first during evolution, with subjective affective reactions following in some species, via the evolution of more elaborate and hierarchical brain mesocorticolimbic circuitry to translate core &#x;liking&#x; reactions into conscious feelings of pleasure (Damasio and Carvalho, ).

Objective hedonic reactions

A useful example of an objective hedonic reaction is the orofacial affective expression of &#x;liking&#x; elicited by tastes in newborn human infants (Steiner, ). Positive taste &#x;liking&#x; versus negative &#x;disgust&#x; expressions can be elicited on the first post-natal day (Figure 1). Sweet tastes elicit positive hedonic &#x;liking&#x; expressions comprising relaxed facial muscles and a contented licking of the lips, whereas bitter tastes elicit &#x;disgust&#x; expressions. Homologous &#x;liking&#x; orofacial expressions can be elicited also in apes and monkeys, and even in rats and mice (e.g., rhythmic tongue protrusions and lateral lip licking to sweetness versus gapes and headshakes to bitterness)(Berridge, ; Grill and Norgren, a; Steiner et al., ). The basic sensorimotor circuitry of these affective expressions resides in the brainstem (Grill and Norgren, b; Steiner, ), but such affective expressions are not mere brainstem reflexes, but rather are hierarchically controlled by forebrain structures. Forebrain circuitry exerts powerful descending control over brainstem and behavioral output. One consequence is that &#x;liking&#x; expressions elicited by a given taste are appropriately modulated physiologically by relevant appetite versus satiety states (Cabanac and Lafrance, ; Kaplan et al., ) as well as associatively by learned preferences and aversions. (Delamater et al., ). ost strikingly, &#x;liking&#x; reactions are powerfully controlled by discrete neural manipulations located in several limbic forebrain structures, as will be discussed (Castro and Berridge, ; Mahler et al., ; Peciña and Berridge, ; Smith and Berridge, ).

&#x;Liking&#x; facial expressions also belong to the consummatory class of motivated behaviors, which typically occurs after an initial appetitive phase of flexible seeking behavior (Craig, ; Sherrington, ). Those hedonic reactions co-occur with several other ingestive consummatory reactions, including voluntary consumption of food, the microstructure of consumption movements (often measured as spout-lick patterns by lickometer in animal studies) and the simple brainstem decision to swallow food in the mouth. But consummatory reactions are highly heterogeneous. In particular, affective reactions taste reactivity patterns have a uniquely specific relation to the hedonic evaluation of taste &#x;liking&#x;, and sometimes for that reason dissociate from all other consummatory reactions (Berridge, ). Dissociation is most commonly induced by manipulations that alter motivational (i.e., &#x;wanting&#x;) but not hedonic aspects (&#x;liking&#x;) of the value of a food incentive. For example, dopamine suppressions reduce the incentive value of sweetness similar to sucrose dilution, as reflected in changes in lickometer measures of ingestive microstructure (Galistu and D'Aquila, ; Smith, ) as well as suppressing appetitive seeking and sometimes food intake (Wise and Raptis, ). Yet, taste reactivity &#x;liking&#x; expressions are not diminished by such pharmacological dopamine blockade (Peciña et al., ), nor even by complete destruction of mesolimbic dopamine projections. Such dissociations have indicated that dopamine is not actually needed for the hedonic impact of food pleasure, but rather only for their incentive motivation value, as described further below.

Subjective versus objective levels of hedonic reaction

As mentioned above, to avoid confusion it is useful to use &#x;liking&#x; (in quotes) to specifically refer to behavioral or neural hedonic reactions, whether or not those objective &#x;liking&#x; reactions are accompanied by a corresponding conscious liking or feeling of pleasure (which may require additional neural mechanisms). A similar distinction applies to conscious wanting versus the mesolimbic motivation process of incentive salience or &#x;wanting&#x; and its objective consequences. The subjective versus objective distinction is based also on evidence that even in humans the two forms of hedonic reaction can be independently measured. For example, objective hedonic &#x;liking&#x; reactions can sometimes occur alone and unconsciously in ordinary people without any subjective pleasure feeling at all, at least in particular situations (e.g., evoked by subliminally brief or mild affective stimuli) (Childress et al., ; Fischman and Foltin, ; Winkielman et al., ). Unconscious &#x;liking&#x; reactions still effectively change goal-directed human behavior, though those changes may remain undetected or be misinterpreted even by the person who has them (Bargh et al., ; Childress et al., ; Pessiglione et al., ; Winkielman et al., ). More commonly, &#x;liking&#x; reactions occur together with conscious feelings of liking, and provide a hedonic signal input to cognitive ratings and subjective feelings. However, dissociations between the two levels of hedonic reaction can still sometimes occur in normal people due to the susceptibility of subjective ratings of liking to cognitive distortions by framing effects, or as a consequence of theories concocted by people to explain how they think they should feel (Gilbert and Wilson, ; Schooler and Mauss, ). For example, framing effects can cause two people exposed to the same stimulus to report different subjective ratings, if one of them had a wider range of previously experienced hedonic intensities (e.g., pains of childbirth or severe injury) (Bartoshuk, ). In short, there is a difference between how people feel and report subjectively versus how they objectively respond with neural or behavioral affective reactions. Subjective ratings are not always more accurate about hedonic impact than objective hedonic reactions, and the latter can be measured independently of the former.

Mapping pleasure in the brain

The experience of one pleasure often seems very different from another. Eating delicious foods, romantic or sexual pleasures, addictive drugs, listening to music, or seeing a loved one: each feels unique. The only psychological feature in common would seem that all are pleasant. However, the difference in one&#x;s subjective experiences is not necessarily a good guide to the underlying neural mechanisms. Those neural mechanisms may overlap to a surprising degree.

Over the last decades, a growing set of results from neuroimaging studies have suggested that many diverse rewards activate a shared or overlapping brain system: a &#x;common currency&#x; reward network of interacting brain regions. Pleasures of food, sex, addictive drugs, friends and loved ones, music, art, and even sustained states of happiness can produce strikingly similar patterns of brain activity (Cacioppo et al., ; Georgiadis and Kringelbach, ; Kringelbach et al., ; Parsons et al., ; Salimpoor et al., ; Vartanian and Skov, ; Veldhuizen et al., ; Vuust and Kringelbach, ; Xu et al., ; Zeki and Romaya, ). These shared reward networks include anatomical regions of prefrontal cortex, including portions of orbitofrontal, insula, and anterior cingulate cortices, as well as often subcortical limbic structures such as nucleus accumbens (NAc), ventral pallidum (VP), and amygdala (shown for rats and humans in Figure 2). An implication of the &#x;common currency&#x; hypothesis is that insights into brain hedonic substrates gained by experiments using one kind of pleasure, such as food &#x;liking&#x;, may apply to many other pleasures too.

Three-dimensional comparison of hedonic sites in rat brain (left) and human brain (right)

A. Rat brain shows hedonic hotspots (red) and coldspots (blue) in coronal, sagittal, horizontal planes and in 3D fronto-lateral perspective view (clockwise from top left). B. Human brain shows extrapolation of rat causal hotspots to analogous human sites in NAc and VP (red), and shows fMRI coding sites for positive affective reactions in green (from text). Human views are also in coronal, sagittal, horizontal and 3D perspective (clockwise from top left of B). The tentative functional networks between the different hotspots and coldspots have been added to give an impression of the topology of a pleasure network. The functional connection lines are not meant to imply direct anatomical projections between two connected structures, but rather a functional network in mediating hedonic &#x;liking&#x; reactions and subjective pleasure ratings. Abbreviations: VP, ventral pallidum; NAc, nucleus accumbens; PBN, parabrachial nucleus; mOFC, medial orbitofrontal cortex; lOFC, lateral orbitofrontal cortex; midOFC, mid-anterior orbitofrontal cortex; dACC, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex; rACC, rostral anterior cingulate cortex; PAG, periaqueductal gray.

Admittedly fMRI measures have limits in spatial and temporal resolution that might miss small or fast differences among neural subsystems that encode particular rewards. It remains possible that more fine-grained spatial and temporal multivariate pattern analysis techniques (Haynes and Rees, ; King and Dehaene, ) will identify subsets of limbic neural circuitry particular to just one type of reward (Chikazoe et al., ). Consistent with this, subtle differences may be found in neuronal firing in animal studies between different sensory rewards, such as tasty foods versus addictive drugs (though some neural differences may be due to accompanying confounds, such as different movements required to obtain the different rewards, or sensory accompaniments, rather than to unique reward encoding per se) (Cameron and Carelli, ). Still, so far, the balance of evidence suggests rather massive overlap between neural systems that mediate rewards of different types. The overlap is far more extensive than many might have expected based on the subjective differences in experiences.

One human brain site that appears especially linked to pleasure in neuroimaging studies is in orbitofrontal cortex, particularly in a mid-anterior subregion (Figures 2 and and3).3). Other medial regions of orbitofrontal cortex, middle anterior regions of insula cortex, and ventromedial regions of prefrontal cortex cortices also correlate with subjective pleasure ratings, but many of these other regions appear to be more concerned with monitoring or predicting reward values than with generating the pleasure per se (Georgiadis and Kringelbach, ; Kahnt et al., ; Kringelbach, ; Kringelbach et al., ; O&#x;Doherty, ; Schoenbaum and Roesch, ; Veldhuizen et al., ; Vuust and Kringelbach, ).

Hedonic coding in the human orbitofrontal cortex (OFC)

In humans, the orbitofrontal cortex is an important hub for pleasure coding, albeit heterogeneous, where different sub-regions are involved in different aspects of hedonic processing. A) Neuroimaging investigations have found differential activity to rewards depending on context in three subregions: the medial OFC (mOFC), mid-anterior OFC (midOFC) and lateral OFC (lOFC). B) A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies showing task-related activity in the OFC demonstrated different functional roles for these three sub-regions. In particular, the midOFC appears to best code the subjective experience of pleasure such as food and sex (orange), while mOFC monitors the valence, learning and memory of reward values (green area and round blue dots). However, unlike the midOFC, activity in the mOFC is not sensitive to reward devaluation and thus may not so faithfully track pleasure. In contrast, the lOFC region is active when punishers force a behavioural change (purple and orange triangles). Furthermore, the meta-analysis showed a posterior-axis of reward complexity such that more abstract rewards (such as money) will engage more anterior regions to more sensory rewards (such as taste). C) Further investigations into the role of the OFC on the spontaneous dynamics during rest found broadly similar sub-divisions in terms of functional connectivity (Kahnt et al., ) with an optimal hierarchical clustering of four to six OFC regions. This included medial (1), posterior central (2), central (3) and lateral (4&#x;6) clusters with the latter spanning an anterior-posterior gradient (bottom of Fig 3B), and connected to different cortical and subcortical regions (top of Figure 3B). Taken together, both the task-related and resting-state activity provides evidence for a significant role of the OFC in a common currency network. It is also compatible with a relatively simple model where primary sensory areas feed reinforcer identity to the OFC where it is combined to form multi-modal representations and assigned a reward value to help guide adaptive behaviour (Kringelbach and Rolls, ). Images in A are reproduced from (Kringelbach et al., ; Kringelbach et al., ).

It is important to remember that neuroimaging studies are correlational in nature rather than causal, and that the physiological bases of underlying signals (such as the BOLD signal measured with fMRI) are only partly understood (Winawer et al., ). Interpreting correlational signals is complicated. Some correlational neuroimaging activity may of course reflect causal mechanisms for pleasure, while other activity may be a consequence, rather than cause. That is because many brain regions that become active during a normal pleasure may not actually generate that pleasure per se, but rather activate as a step to causally generating their own different functions, such as cognitive appraisal, memory, attention, and decision making about the pleasant event.

However, the mid-anterior subregion of orbitofrontal cortex in particular does appear to track subjective pleasure more accurately than most other limbic regions (Figure 3). One of the strongest tests for pleasure coding is to hold the pleasant stimulus constant across successive exposures, but vary its hedonic impact by altering other input factors such as relevant physiological states. For example, evidence suggests that mid-anterior orbitofrontal activity tracks sensory satiety, involving selective declines in the subjective pleasantness of a given food&#x;s taste after consuming a lot of it, compared to another food which is not devalued (Gottfried et al., ; Kringelbach et al., ). Tracking a change in pleasure of a stimulus is the strongest possible correlational evidence, because it shows the activity is not coding mere sensory features (e.g., sweetness) or other stable confounds. The same region of OFC has also been implicated in the encoding pleasures of sexual orgasm, drugs, and music (Georgiadis and Kringelbach, ; Kringelbach, ; Kringelbach et al., ; Salimpoor et al., ; Veldhuizen et al., ; Vuust and Kringelbach, ). Subcortically, there is evidence from other animals that such selective hedonic changes also may be tracked by activity in nucleus accumbens and ventral pallidum (Krause et al., ; Loriaux et al., ; Roitman et al., ; Tindell et al., ).

Some studies also indicate lateralization of affect representation, often as lateralized hemispheric differences in coding positive versus negative valence. Most notably, the left hemisphere of prefrontal cortex often has been implicated more in positive affect than right hemisphere (Davidson, ). For example, individuals who give higher ratings of subjective well-being may have higher activity in left than right prefrontal cortex, and activity of left subcortical striatum also may be more tightly linked to pleasantness ratings than right-side (Kuhn and Gallinat, ; Lawrence et al., ; Price and Harmon-Jones, ). However, other studies have found more equal or bilateral activity patterns, and so the precise role of lateralization in pleasure still needs further clarification.

An important caveat of human neuroimaging studies is that these have traditionally compared a hedonic activation with a baseline at rest. Recently, it has become clear that the brain is never truly resting but rather spontaneously active and constantly switching between different resting state networks (Cabral et al., ). The switching between different networks depend on the state of the brain, and so one way to think about the pleasure system is to facilitate the state transition between different points in the pleasure cycle to optimize survival. Plausibly, the so-called default mode network may play an essential role in this, and thus problems in orchestrating the state transitions may manifest as anhedonia in affective disorders (Kringelbach and Berridge, ). With advanced computational modelling of human neuroimaging data this is now becoming a testable hypothesis (Cabral et al., ). New efforts have given birth to computational neuropsychiatry as a way to discover novel biomarkers for affective states and in neuropsychiatric disorders, and potentially help rebalance brain networks (Deco and Kringelbach, ).

Mapping brain pleasure generators?

Mapping causal generators of pleasure in the brain is a challenge because it can require invasive brain manipulations, needed to establish evidence for causation, which are ruled out by legitimate ethical constraints in human studies. However, evidence from animal studies is revealing a network of hedonic hotspots that causally enhance &#x;liking&#x; reactions to pleasant stimuli, and coldspots that diminish the &#x;liking&#x; reactions (Figure 2).

A useful starting distinction is between causation of loss versus gain of function. In loss of function, lesions or neural dysfunctions reveal mechanisms that are necessary for normal function. In gain of function, neurobiological stimulations reveal mechanisms that are sufficient to cause higher levels of hedonic impact. While some neural structures mediate both forms of causation for hedonic function, other neural mechanisms may mediate only one: for example, able to produce gains of function that enhance pleasure reactions without being needed for normal pleasure. Brain structures able to cause gains in hedonic function may be more widely distributed than structures needed for normal pleasure reactions, which are more anatomically restricted and subcortically weighted. Further, both forms of causation may be more restricted than the coding activity revealed by neuroimaging correlations with pleasure described above.

As illustration, entire limbic regions of human prefrontal cortex appear surprisingly unnecessary for the causal generation of normal pleasure. For example, the surgical procedure of prefrontal lobotomy, performed on thousands of patients during the s, removed or disconnected most of their prefrontal lobe (Valenstein, ). Yet lobotomy patients retained most hedonic feelings as far as could be discerned (albeit showing impairments in cognitive judgment), as do other human patients with similarly large prefrontal cortex lesions arising from stroke, tumor or injury (Damasio, ; Szczepanski and Knight, ). A dramatic recent report confirmed that even more massive cortical damage, destroying not only prefrontal orbitofrontal and ventromedial cortex but also frontal insula and ventral anterior cingulate cortex (plus hippocampus and amygdala in the rostral temporal lobe), left intact normal behavioral affective reactions to preferred social partners or frightening syringes, and even verbal hedonic reports such as I have a strong feeling of happiness, that we are here together working on these wonderful games (Damasio et al., ).

Stark examples of subcortical causation of normal hedonic reactions in people also include hydranencephalic children, who essentially lack a telencephalic forebrain and have virtually no cortex, yet may still show complex emotional responses to social caregivers and music. For example, Shewmon et al. described complex behavioral hedonic reactions in hydranencephalic children, such as in a 6-year old boy born with congenital absence of cerebral tissue rostral to the thalamus, except for small mesial temporal-lobe remnants (p. ), who still smiled when spoken to and giggled when played with. These human interactions were much more intense than, and qualitatively different from, his positive reactions to favorite toys and music. (p. ) (Shewmon et al., ). Similarly, Merker reported that other hydranencephalic children "express pleasure by smiling and laughter, and aversion by &#x;fussing&#x;, arching of the back and crying (in many gradations), their faces being animated by these emotional states. A familiar adult can employ this responsiveness to build up play sequences predictably progressing from smiling, through giggling, to laughter and great excitement on the part of the child."(p. 79)(Merker, ). Such cases of human emotional reaction without (hardly any) cortex indicate that subcortical structures may be surprisingly competent to generate many normal hedonic reactions, and are consistent with many animal studies.

Causal hedonic hotspots for hedonic enhancements

Yet hedonic gains of function can be produced by neural events in several forebrain structures, resulting in intense pleasure reactions. Animal affective neuroscience studies have recently identified a network for generating hedonic enhancement of &#x;liking&#x; reactions, embedded as a set of small hedonic hotspots distributed among several limbic structures throughout the brain, ranging from cortex to brainstem. Each hotspot can specifically amplify orofacial &#x;liking&#x; expressions elicited by sweetness in rats, when neurochemically stimulated by an appropriate drug microinjection. Hedonic hotspots have been found in subcortical forebrain nucleus accumbens and connected ventral pallidum, in the brainstem parabrachial nucleus of the pons, and may now be emerging in limbic areas of prefrontal cortex, including orbitofrontal cortex and insula (Castro and Berridge, ; Castro et al., ; Ho and Berridge, ; Peciña and Berridge, ; Smith and Berridge, ; Soderpalm and Berridge, ).

The size of hedonic hotspots mapped so far is each about 1 cubic millimeter in volume in rats (which might be extrapolated to a cubic centimeter in humans, if proportional to brain size). By comparison, each structure that contains a hotspot is much larger. For example, the entire nucleus accumbens comprises nearly 10 mm3 in rats, but its opioid hedonic hotspot located in the rostrodorsal quadrant of medial shell constitutes only 10% of total NAc volume (and about 30% of volume of medial shell; shown in Figures 1 &#x; 2) (Castro and Berridge, ; Peciña and Berridge, ). In other words, as far as is known, nearly 90% of the remaining NAc may lack capacity to enhance &#x;liking&#x; reactions, even for mu opioid stimulation.

In more detail, inside the rostrodorsal hotspot of medial shell in NAc, mu opioid stimulation via agonist microinjections can at least double the hedonic impact of sucrose, as reflected in more &#x;liking&#x; reactions (Peciña and Berridge, ; Smith et al., ). Somewhat surprisingly, delta opioid stimulation or even kappa opioid stimulation also in the same NAc hotspot will similarly enhance hedonic impact of sweetness (Castro and Berridge, ). At other sites in NAc medial shell, all three types of opioid stimulations fail to enhance &#x;liking&#x; reactions, and indeed all oppositely suppress &#x;liking&#x; reactions at a &#x;coldspot&#x; site in the caudal half of medial shell. That localization suggests the NAc rostrodorsal hotspot is really quite unique as a mechanism for gating hedonic gain of function. Independently, a unique role for the NAc hotspot was confirmed using conditioned place preference tests: mu, kappa and delta stimulations all establish positive preferences for a place paired with the microinjections in hotspot, but not at other sites in NAc medial shell (Castro and Berridge, ). Beyond opioid signals, endocannabinoid stimulation by microinjections of anandamide similarly enhances &#x;liking&#x; reactions in an overlapping subregion of NAc medial shell (Mahler et al., ). The anatomical overlap between opioid and endocannabinoid hotspots in NAc raises the possibility that the circuitry in the same hotspot may largely mediate both neurochemical forms of pleasure enhancement.

What makes the NAc hotspot so special? The full answer remains for future, but some insights are emerging from recent reports that the NAc hotspot in rostrodorsal medial shell has unique neuroanatomical features, and also unique neurochemical features, different from other subregions of medial shell and NAc core (Britt and McGehee, ; Kupchik and Kalivas, ; Thompson and Swanson, ; Zahm et al., ).

Beyond NAc, the ventral pallidum (VP) is a major target of NAc projections. The VP also contains its own hotspot located at posterior end (Ho and Berridge, ; Smith and Berridge, ). The VP hotspot similarly is about 1mm3 in volume, constituting less than one-half of the total VP. In the VP hotspot, either mu opioid or orexin-A stimulating microinjections more than double the level of &#x;liking&#x; reactions elicited by sweetness (Ho and Berridge, ; Smith and Berridge, ). Conversely, more rostrally in VP, a hedonic coldspot of similar volume exists where mu opioid stimulation oppositely reduces sweetness &#x;liking&#x; (Smith and Berridge, ). Recent optogenetic studies have also begun to help confirm this hedonic gain of function capacity, by indicating that optogenetic excitation (channelrhodopsin) of neurons within the VP hotspot can double the number of &#x;liking&#x; reactions to sweetness (Castro and Berridge, ). Further optogenetic confirmations would provide valuable independent validation of the hedonic function of the VP hotspot.

The circuitry connecting hotspots of nucleus accumbens and ventral pallidum remains unclear, and they may not be directly connected. Yet the two hotspots functionally interact to form an integrated circuit. For example, stimulating either hotspot can recruit activation of the other, and mutual recruitment into simultaneous participation appears necessary to enhance &#x;liking&#x; reactions, in the sense that blocking opioid activation in either hotspot completely prevents mu opioid stimulation of the other one from enhancing &#x;liking&#x; (Smith and Berridge, ; Smith et al., ).

Hotspots at top and bottom of the brain?

In the prefrontal cortex, recent evidence indicates that orbitofrontal cortex and insula cortex may each contain their own additional hotspots (Castro et al., ). In specific subregions of each area, either opioid-stimulating or orexin- stimulating microinjections appear to enhance the number of &#x;liking&#x; reactions elicited by sweetness, similar to NAc and VP hotspots (Castro et al., ). Successful confirmation of hedonic hotspots in orbitofrontal cortex or insula would be important, and possibly relevant to the orbitofrontal mid-anterior site mentioned earlier that especially tracks the subjective pleasure of foods in humans (Georgiadis et al., ; Kringelbach, ; Kringelbach et al., ; Small et al., ; Veldhuizen et al., ).

Finally, in brainstem, a hindbrain site near the parabrachial nucleus of dorsal pons also appears able to contribute to hedonic gains of function (Soderpalm and Berridge, ). A brainstem mechanism for pleasure may seem more surprising than forebrain hotspots to anyone who views brainstem as merely reflexive, but the pontine parabrachial nucleus contributes to taste, pain and many visceral sensations from the body, and has also been suggested to play an important roles in motivation (Wu et al., ) and in human emotion (especially related to the somatic marker hypothesis) (Damasio, ). Further a brainstem contribution to pleasure circuitry is quite consistent with a hierarchical view of brain organization, which would suggest hedonic functions to be reiteratively represented at multiple levels of the brain.

Interaction between hotpot site and neurochemical stimulation

Hotspots generate hedonic enhancement through an interaction between their specific anatomical site and their particular neurochemical state or mode of stimulation. Neither alone is sufficient to enhance &#x;liking&#x;. For example, in the NAc hotspot in rostrodorsal medial shell, microinjections of mu, delta, or kappa opioid agonists all double the &#x;liking&#x; reactions elicited by sucrose taste, as does endocannabinoid stimulation in its overlapping hotspot (Castro and Berridge, ; Mahler et al., ; Peciña and Berridge, ). But in the same NAc hotspot, neither dopamine stimulation or glutamate AMPA blockade alter hedonic &#x;liking&#x; for sucrose at all, even though both elevate &#x;wanting&#x; to eat as effectively as opioid stimulation (Faure et al., ; Smith et al., ). In other words, in the NAc hotspot, the particular neurochemical mode determines whether &#x;liking&#x; for sweetness will be enhanced or not, as well controlling &#x;wanting&#x; to eat. Neurochemical mode is clearly as important as anatomical site. Yet outside the hotspot at other sites in NAc shell, even mu opioid and endocannabinoid stimulations fail to enhance &#x;liking&#x; at all (Castro and Berridge, ; Mahler et al., ; Peciña and Berridge, ). In fact, NAc microinjections of mu, delta or kappa opioid agonists in the posterior hedonic coldspot of shell all oppositely suppress &#x;liking&#x; reactions elicited by sweetness to just half normal levels &#x; even though mu stimulation at that posterior NAc site still enhances cue-triggered &#x;wanting&#x; to obtain reward and stimulates &#x;wanting&#x; to eat as much as in the anterior hotspot (Castro and Berridge, ; Pecina and Berridge, ). Thus anatomical site gates the hedonic effectiveness of those neurochemical modes. Clearly, it is the interaction between hotspot site and mode of neurochemical stimulation that determines hedonic impact.

Ventral pallidum hotspot: sufficient to enhance and needed for normal &#x;liking&#x;

Prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens do share one interesting quirk regarding causation of hedonic impact. Both contain hotspots able to cause gains of hedonic function for intense &#x;liking&#x;, but neither when damaged cause loss of hedonic function: neither reducing positive &#x;liking&#x; reactions nor increasing negative &#x;disgust&#x; reactions. By contrast, the hedonic hotspot of posterior ventral pallidum combines causation for gain of function with necessity for normal baseline levels of &#x;liking&#x;: that necessity is revealed after caudal VP lesion by loss of positive &#x;liking&#x; for sweetness and replacement by intense &#x;negative disgust&#x; reactions (e.g. gapes and headshakes elicited by sucrose) (Cromwell and Berridge, ; Ho and Berridge, ). In short, the posterior VP hotspot appears more crucial than any other known brain site for loss of hedonic function after damage, at least for taste pleasure. Even classic lateral hypothalamic lesions that once were thought to induce intense food disgust (Teitelbaum and Epstein, ), may have done so actually only by additionally damaging the posterior ventral pallidum (Ho and Berridge, ; Smith et al., ).

Besides lesions, temporary pharmacological inactivation in the posterior VP hotspot also causes intense &#x;disgust&#x; (Ho and Berridge, ; Shimura et al., ). By comparison in NAc shell, intense &#x;disgust&#x; is caused by only temporary inactivations (not lesions, suggesting disruptions must act to impair hedonic impact before circuitry compensations can occur), and only in the posterior &#x;coldspot&#x; (not rostrodorsal &#x;hotspot&#x;) (Ho and Berridge, ). That difference between VP and NAc suggests that NAc segregates hedonic gain of function versus loss of function into different anatomical sites of medial shell, whereas the VP hotspot combines both forms of hedonic causation together(Ho and Berridge, ). The VP hotspot thus appears unique among brain sites for hedonic loss of function.

The excessive disgust that follows these VP disruptions may be viewed as a release phenomenon, produced by disinhibition of negative-valenced circuitry in the remaining forebrain diencephalon (Ho and Berridge, ). Similar intense &#x;disgust&#x; and other aversive emotions is also produced by large ablations of the entire telencephalon that include the ventral pallidum as well as other telencephalic forebrain structures, but leave intact the diencephalic hypothalamus and thalamus (Bard, ; Grill and Norgren, b), whereas positive reactivity is spared by lower transections of the brain, such as midbrain decerebration (which eliminates all forebrain circuitry, including NAc, VP and hypothalamus)(Grill and Norgren, b). A disinhibition interpretation also fits a hierarchical view of how pleasure and displeasure are organized in the brain (Hughlings Jackson, ).

Desire to dread: an affective keyboard in NAc shell

The anterior NAc opioid hedonic hotspot and posterior suppressive coldspot fit within a broader anatomical NAc pattern of front-to-back valence organization in shell that generates additional emotions beyond &#x;liking&#x; and &#x;disgust&#x;. This NAc pattern resembles an affective keyboard arranged rostrocaudally within medial shell, which can generate intense desire or even dread as well as hedonic impact (Reynolds and Berridge, ; Richard and Berridge, ) (Figure 4). The keyboard pattern is arranged from anterior to posterior ends of medial shell. At its anterior end, it generates predominantly positive-valenced motivations in response to localized neural events such as microinjections of a GABA agonist (muscimol) or of a glutamate AMPA antagonist (DNQX): eating more than twice normal amounts of food, increasing appetitive seeking for food rewards (Stratford and Kelley, ; Stratford and Wirtshafter, ; Wirtshafter et al., ), inducing a conditioned preference for a place paired with the microinjection, and (for GABA microinjections) even increasing &#x;liking&#x; reactions to sweet tastes (Reynolds and Berridge, ). However, as the microinjection site moves more caudally in NAc shell, appetitive behaviors decline. Instead negative &#x;fearful&#x; behavior becomes increasingly intense, and (for GABA) sweet tastes become also disgusting (Faure et al., ; Ho and Berridge, ; Reynolds and Berridge, ; Richard et al., b).

Affective keyboard in nucleus accumbens for desire and/or dread

Top: A rostrocaudal keyboard pattern of generators in NAc for appetitive versus fearful behaviors, showing consequences of microinjections of either glutamate AMPA antagonist or GABA agonist microinjections at rostrocaudal sites in medial shell. Rostral green sites produced % increases in food consumption (desire only). Caudal red sites generated purely increased fearful reactions at levels up to % over normal (escape attempts, distress calls, defensive bite attempts; spontaneous anti-predator treading/burying e). Photos show examples of antipredator treading/burying behavior elicited by threat stimuli: ground squirrel toward rattlesnake predator, rat toward electric-shock prod in lab. The same antipredator behaviors occurs without any specific threat stimulus after DNQX or muscimol microinjections in posterior NAc: denoted by red dots. Yellow sites released both desire and fearful behaviors in the same rats during the same 1-hr test. Just as a keyboard has many notes, bars reflect the many graded mixtures of affective desire-dread released as microinjection sites move rostrocaudal location in medial shell (appetitive desire to eat at top; fearful dread reactions at bottom). Bottom: Environmental ambience retuned the NAc keyboard. A comfortable &#x;home environment&#x; (the rat&#x;s own home room: dark, quiet, smell and sound of conspecifics in the room) suppressed fearful behaviors, and expanded zone for appetitive behaviors, produced by microinjections that block glutamate AMPA receptors (DNQX). A standard laboratory environment rebalances the keyboard into nearly equal halves for desire versus dread. A stressfully over-stimulating sensory environment (bright lights plus loud rock music) tilted the causal keyboard toward dread, and shrank the zoned that generated appetitive desire. Squirrel photo by Cooke from (Coss and Owings, ). Figure data modified from (Richard et al., a), based on data from (Reynolds and Berridge, ; Richard and Berridge, ).

Of course, several other brain structures, from amygdala to hypothalamus, ventral pallidum or brainstem also known to mediate various aversive emotional reactions, including fear, pain or disgust (Baliki et al., ; LeDoux, ; von dem Hagen et al., ). The amygdala is especially crucial for fear-related learning of passive responses to threats, such as freezing to a Pavlovian cue that predicts footshock (LeDoux, ; Maren et al., ). The posterior NAc instead produces a more active set of fearful coping reactions (Faure et al., ; Reynolds and Berridge, ; Richard et al., b). For example, these include distress calls and frantic escape leaps by a normally tame rat when approached or touched by a human hand, and even defensive bites directed toward the offending hand, as active unconditioned &#x;fearful&#x; responses. Or when left alone after a microinjection, the rat spontaneously often emits &#x;fearful&#x; antipredator reactions that rodents typically use in the wild to defend against natural threats (e.g., defensive burying toward a rattlesnake) (Coss and Owings, ). These defensive reactions are usually targeted toward stimuli the affected rat may perceive as potentially threatening, such as glittering transparent corners of the cage or experimenters visible beyond the transparent wall (Coss and Owings, ; Reynolds and Berridge, ).

Multiple anatomical modules in NAc shell

The number of differently-valenced rostrocaudal keys contained in the nucleus accumbens shell is difficult to estimate, and in practice is defined somewhat arbitrarily by the size of the microinjections used to tap the keyboard. But probably it contains more than two keys corresponding to mere positive vs negative valence: two keys would generate only two outputs, but the NAc shell generates many different incremental outputs of gradual variation depending on precise site. Just as a musical keyboard generates many distinct notes, the rostrocaudal affective keyboard generates multiple distinct quantities of appetitive versus fearful behaviors. For example, as sites move from front to back, intense behaviors become gradually less appetitive, and incrementally more fearful, so that many different ratio mixtures are produced, just as moving a brick along a piano keyboard would generate many different mixtures of notes changing gradually in pitch.

However, a causal caveat may be needed here. To say an appetitive mechanism is densest in the anterior half of NAc shell may really be to say that the anterior half is densest in neural mechanisms which ordinarily inhibit appetitive behavior &#x; and which themselves must be inhibited by the rostral microinjection that produces the intense appetitive behavior. This disinhibition interpretation arises because of the inhibitory nature of the GABAA agonist or glutamate antagonist microinjections that produce the intense behaviors. The drug microinjections either hyperpolarize NAc neurons (i.e., muscimol stimulates GABA receptors) or at least block excitatory depolarizations of NAc neurons (i.e., via DNQX blocks glutamate AMPA receptors).

Both drugs produce similar motivation keyboard patterns of intense appetitive-fearful behaviors when microinjected in medial shell, and the GABA agonist adds a corresponding hedonic keyboard of &#x;liking-disgust&#x; effects (Faure et al., ; Richard and Berridge, ). A disinhibition interpretation suggests that reduced activity of NAc projection neurons, which themselves release mostly GABA, would release or disinhibit recipient neurons in target structures into relative excitation (e.g., in VP, hypothalamus, or ventral tegmentum) (Carlezon and Thomas, ; Meredith et al., ; Roitman et al., ). Target excitations could be the final active mechanism to produce intense motivations. Output projections from particular rostrocaudal sites in NAc shell appear partly segregated from each other in target structures (Thompson and Swanson, ; Zahm et al., ), which might help tune the valence of intense desire/dread motivations produced at different NAc sites. Although some contrary evidence suggests that local NAc excitations also generate motivated behaviors (Britt et al., ; Taha and Fields, ), this disinhibition hypothesis at least does potentially account for many features of NAc in motivation (Carlezon and Thomas, ), including the NAc keyboard production of &#x;desire&#x; versus &#x;fear&#x;.

Retuning the affective keyboard

Strikingly, the valence of desire-dread motivations generated by the NAc keyboard is not necessarily fixed by anatomical location, but can be powerfully retuned psychologically for many sites by emotional factors such as the valenced ambience of an environment (Figure 4). At least, dramatic psychological retuning occurs for the glutamate-related DNQX gradient that merely blocks local NAc excitation (Reynolds and Berridge, ; Richard and Berridge, ). By comparison, the GABA-related muscimol gradient is more resistant to retuning, perhaps because it involves stronger neuronal NAc hyperpolarization (Richard et al., b). Retuning can completely reverse the valence generated at a site from desire to dread, or back from dread to desire. For example, the fear-generating zone of caudal shell expands in a stressfully bright and loud environment to invade rostral shell, while simultaneously shrinking the desire-generating zone to only the far-rostral tip of medial shell (Reynolds and Berridge, ; Richard and Berridge, ). Conversely, a quiet home-like environment (which rats prefer) causes the NAc keyboard to expand its rostral desire-generating zone into the caudal half of shell, and shrink the fear-generating zone into merely the far-caudal tip. Such remapping can actually flip many intermediate sites of shell into releasing opposite motivations in the different environments.

Speculatively, it can be hypothesized that some pathological human conditions might induce more permanent retuning of NAc valence generators. For example, post-traumatic stress disorder might persistently retune NAc generation in a fearful direction in human patients, similarly to a stressful ambience. Conversely, human addiction and mesolimbic sensitization might retune NAc generators in an appetitive direction, potentiating desire for addicted rewards. These possibilities could be explored by future research.

For the glutamatergic keyboard in rats, the neurobiological mechanism of psychological retuning appears to rewire local neurobiological modes of neurochemical activation within the local NAc microdomain. For example, generation of &#x;fear&#x; behaviors by NAc AMPA blockade requires endogenous dopamine activity at both D1 and D2 receptors simultaneously within the local microinjection site; the defensive motivation can be prevented by adding an antagonist for either dopamine receptor to the eliciting DNQX microinjection (Faure et al., ; Richard and Berridge, ). By contrast, generation of appetitive desire, even at the same NAc site, requires only D1 activity &#x; not D2 activity (Richard and Berridge, ). That pattern suggests that direct and indirect output paths of NAc may have different roles in this desire-dread generation. Dopamine D1 receptors occur mostly on NAc neurons belonging to the &#x;direct&#x; output path that includes a projection directly to ventral tegmentum, whereas D2 receptors occur mostly on neurons belonging to the &#x;indirect&#x; output path that projects only to VP and hypothalamus (Humphries and Prescott, ). Thus both paths may be equally important in producing the intense &#x;fearful&#x; reaction, whereas positive &#x;desire&#x; generation may be dominated by the direct path (Richard and Berridge, ). If so, that would be consistent with suggestions from others that a NAc D1 direct path dominates in appetitive motivation (Xiu et al., ).

Finally, NAc keyboard tuning is regulated by corticolimbic top-down inputs from prefrontal limbic cortex (Richard and Berridge, ). For example, raising local cortical excitations in infralimbic cortex, a medial prefrontal region homologous to human subgenual anterior cingulate cortex (Area 25), broadly suppressed the intensity of motivations otherwise produced by simultaneous NAc microinjections, regardless of valence (Richard and Berridge, ). By comparison, excitation of the orbitofrontal cortex tilted valence in a positive desire direction: at least in the sense of expanding the appetitive zone that generates eating into caudal areas of NAc that otherwise produce negative &#x;fear&#x; reactions (Richard and Berridge, ). Thus corticolimbic regulation adjusts both the intensity and valence of motivations produced by NAc circuitry.

Pruning false candidates: Mesolimbic dopamine and &#x;pleasure electrodes&#x;?

Beyond identifying brain mechanisms that cause subjective feelings of pleasure or objective hedonic reactions, progress in affective neuroscience is also aided by pruning away previous candidates for pleasure generators that have failed to live up to their initial hedonic promise. In our view, two of the most famous brain candidates for pleasure mechanisms featured in textbooks of the past few decades turn out in the end to lack sufficient evidence needed to maintain their hedonic claim: 1) mesolimbic dopamine systems that are activated by many reward-related stimuli, and 2) most so-called &#x;pleasure electrodes&#x; for deep brain stimulation that supported behavioral self-administration (i.e., animals or people were willing to work to stimulate the electrodes, such as by pressing a button). As discussed next, our view is that neither dopamine nor most &#x;pleasure electrodes&#x; actually caused hedonic reactions or pleasure after all, but rather more specifically increased motivation components of reward such as incentive salience, producing &#x;wanting&#x;, without causing &#x;liking&#x; or true hedonic impact.

Mesolimbic dopamine and the (an)hedonia hypothesis

The mesolimbic dopamine system has been the most famous neurochemical candidate in the past half century for a pleasure generator in the brain. The mesolimbic system contains dopamine neurons originating in or near the ventral tegmental area (VTA) of the midbrain, which chiefly ascend to the NAc or ventral striatum, as well as to amygdala, prefrontal cortex and neostriatum. Mesolimbic dopamine systems clearly do play an important role in reward, but that role may not be as hedonic as once thought.

The idea that dopamine was a mechanism for pleasure is known as the &#x;dopamine hedonia&#x; or &#x;dopamine pleasure&#x; hypothesis, and was originally proposed by Roy Wise: dopamine junctions represent a synaptic way station&#x;where sensory inputs are translated into the hedonic messages we experience as pleasure, euphoria or &#x;yumminess&#x;. (Wise, ) (p. 94). Conversely, the &#x;dopamine pleasure hypothesis&#x; postulated that reduction of dopamine neurotransmission caused loss of pleasure. This inverse hypothesis is known as the &#x;dopamine anhedonia hypothesis&#x; (Ettenberg and McFarland, ; Hnasko et al., ; Smith, ; Wise and Colle, ; Wise et al., ).

However, today relatively few neuroscientists who study dopamine in reward appear to assert in print that dopamine causes pleasure. Even original proponents are no longer so enthusiastic. For example, by the mids Wise had retracted the dopamine hedonia hypothesis: he was quoted to say I no longer believe that the amount of pleasure felt is proportional to the amount of dopamine floating around in the brain (p) (Wickelgren, ), and more recently concluded that pleasure is not a necessary correlate of dopamine elevations (p)(Wise, ).

The decline in advocacy of the dopamine pleasure hypothesis stems from of a series of problems that arose in the past two decades. The first problem specifically applied to the anhedonia versions that posited loss of pleasure. Evidence began to emerge that loss of dopamine doesn&#x;t necessarily reduce pleasure after all. For example, in rats even near complete destruction of nigrostriatal and mesolimbic dopamine neurons to approximately 1% normal levels, via extensive 6-hydroxydopamine neurotoxin lesions, turns out to leave orofacial &#x;liking&#x; reactions to sweetness completely intact and unimpaired (

Источник: [manicapital.com]
, Quite A Box of Tricks v1.8-SHOCK serial key or number

Büyük Beden Moda: Ay Şu Moda!

quite a box of tricks, quite a box of tricks download, quite a box of tricks i serial, quite a box of tricks acrobat plugin, quite a box of tricks mac, quite a bag of tricks deviantart, tricks to quit smoking, this trickster is quite a nuisance in the navajo cosmology, quite a box of tricks free download, quite a box of tricks serial, quite box of tricks, quite box of tricks download

Download Quite A Box Of Tricks 8l ->>->>->> manicapital.com



Fix: Fixed in version c. Download now! Cannot see "i" pointer tool button - Acrobat Macintosh. In Acrobat 8 on Mac OS X, you may not Quite A Box Of Tricks is available for download. This is a fully working version, which will mark your work with a large X as long as it is run in demonstration mode.. Download Quite A Box Of Tricks - Quite A Box Of Tricks is a plug-in for Adobe s Acrobat from Quite Software. Updated: October 8, ; platform: Windows All.. Downloads: Download Quite Revealing Serial Number c Quite A Box Of Tricks i Quite Revealing i Unidentified developer Quite A Box Of Tricks i h Quite A Box of Tricks vSHOCK :: :: Quite imposing plus Quite A Box of Tricks Download Software Quite A Box Of Tricks is manual download preceding is labelled together with audi a4 1 8 fuel VW Passat t Fuel pump relay COUPLE TRICKS . Identifying and legend fuse box Audi A4 time to know more not quite this book. subsequent to you have completed content of [PDF], you can in fact realize how importance of a book,.. Join Date: Nov ; Posts: 23; Thanks: 8: Thanked 33 Times in 10 Posts Quite A Box Of Tricks is a plug-in for Adobes Acrobat from Quite Software. Only the registered members can see the download links/content.. Aktuelle Demos & Downloads. Mit der neuen Version wird eine Kompatibilitt zu Acrobat 8 hergestellt. Quite A Box Of Tricks ist in Deutsch verfgbar.. A4 8E Audi Concert service menu Download Audi A4 service & Audi 8E: A4 B6 T Timing belt removal Thomas P.O. Box Kingsway Burnaby, BC in Audi A2, A3 8L, A4 B6, A6 C5, TT 8N. Audi A4 B6 / B7 (tips & tricks) Climatronic zones synchronisation It is not quite the important matter that you can.. Download Quite A Box Of Tricks 8. 21 Janvier 0. quite a box of tricks, quite a box of tricks i serial, quite a box of tricks download, quite a box of tricks PLEASE ALL UPLOAD MORE SOFTWARE FOR FREE DOWNLOAD!!! manicapital.com pennismen Quite Support - Old Macintosh systems. Mac OS X on Intel. Acrobat 8 will run Quite Imposing Plus or later; or Quite A Box Of Tricks or Quite Revealing or later. You can allow Snow Leopard to download and install Rosetta, or install quite a box of tricks MAC - PDF & & . manicapital.com Quite A Box Of Tricks: Overview Latest release i (Nov ) Quite A Box Of Tricks is a plug-in Quite A Box of Tricks vSHOCK serial number download.. Volkswagen Jetta, Golf, GTI Service Manual: l Turbo, l TDI, Parts Catalogue, Fault codes FUSE BOX DIAGRAM Volkswagen Golf Repair Download Volkswagen VW Golf,Jetta,R32 Official Factory Repair Manual I've looked quite a few places for a free A5 pdf service manual and can't find Quite A Box Of Tricks h Activation Code. Quite A Box Of Tricks is a plug-in for Adobe's Acrobat from Quite Software. Quite A Box Of Tricks. Download Quite well as download lead vauxhall zafira diesel engine diagram Vauxhall Zafira Engine Diagram List of owner submitted problems and fixes for the Vauxhall Zafira - "The Vauxhall Zafira is a good family car, but with quite Fuse box location and diagrams: Opel / Vauxhall Zafira B () See more on our website engine scheme diagram, images wiring harness diagram, images fuse box . 98 Honda Civic Radio Wiring Diagram Download - Honda Civic Stereo honda civic 4 door lx ka 5at solenoid parts now engine mounts 1 8l mt Honda Civic now is powered by L, L and L engine mated to CVT or 6-speed.. Overview Login to Download Sample (MiB). Downloads Contains ability to register a top-level exception handler (often used as anti-debugging trick).. This is a soft file book, fittingly you can download it by purchasing to get the soft file. But, we have the funds for you the free information not quite [Epub] Nissan The QG engine is a L (1, cc), L (1, cc), L (1, cc) and L Expert | Posted in Tips & Tricks on Tuesday, October 27th, at pm.

c36ade0fd8

Outlook For Mac Removing An Inbox
Robert Lowie Historia De La Etnologia Pdf Downloadl
Light At The End Of The Tunnel Paul manicapital.com
Driver Genius 12 Serial Codeinstmanksl

Stingray Herunterladen Schl Sselgenerator 64 Bits DE
Dfi de fvrier, question subsidiaire : et si je commence le mois dans le rouge
here s to gettin fucked manicapital.com
Anal sex guy movies free.
Activation BIM Docs Keygen

Источник: [manicapital.com]
Quite A Box of Tricks v1.8-SHOCK serial key or number

Quite A Box Of Tricks

quite a box of tricks, quite a box of tricks i serial, quite a box of tricks download, quite a box of tricks mac, quite a bag of tricks deviantart, this trickster is quite a nuisance in the navajo cosmology, tricks to quit smoking, quite a box of tricks acrobat plugin, quite a box of tricks free download, quite a box of tricks acrobat dc, quite box of tricks, quite box of tricks download



QuikQuak RaySpace VST :: :: 50 QuikQuak RaySpace VST Quite A Box of Tricks vSHOCK :: :: Quite imposing plus Quite A Box Of Tricks i h Quite A Box Of Tricks i h Quite A Box Of Trickspitstoppdf Quite A BoxPDFquite imposing plusQI[] Quite Imposing plus d Quite A Box Of Tricks: Overview. Latest release i (Nov ). Quite A Box Of Tricks is a plug-in for Adobe's Acrobat from Quite Software, for Macintosh Quite A Box Of Tricks c Crack, Serial &. Welcome to Crack Serial Keygen! crack serial keygen searches the internet to help you find what you Quite A Box of Tricks. Download Software. Quite A Box Of Tricks is available for download. This is a fully working version, which will mark your work with a large Security features- protect PDF files by a password. Device independent color PDF only supported RGB, which left quite some room for improvement. Notes [] Quite A Box Of Tricks e - PDF & & .. Quite a Box of Tricks i . Quite A Box Of Tricks Adobe Acrobat x, x x Standard Professional, Quite A Box Of Tricks(PDF) vi ,Quite A BoxPDF Quite A Box Of Tricks i h Quite A Box Of Tricks MAC/WIN. Quite A Box Of Tricks MAC/WIN. Vrobce. QUITE. Kd, QSCA Part No. Status. Dostupnost. nen skladem.. Quite A Box of Tricks PC|MAC - PDF & & .. Quite A Box of Tricks Download Software Quite A Box Of Tricks is available for download. This is a fully working version, which will mark your work with a large X Quite A Box Of Tricks is a plug-in for Adobe's Acrobat from Quite Software, for Macintosh (including Mac OS X) and Windows. Quite A Box Of Quite A Box Of Tricks h, Quite A Box Of Tricks - Quite A Box Of Tricks is a plug-in for Adobe?s Acrobat from Quite Software wurde und wird zuknftig nicht an aktuelle Acrobat Versionen oder Betriebssysteme angepasst. Quite A Box Of Tricks i MacOSX Deutsch (Universal Binary).. Quite a Box of Tricks suite of tools for Acrobat enable you to perform operations on PDF files that are usually difficult, time consuming, or expensive.. Quite A Box Of Tricks is an Adobe Acrobat plug-in that lets you manipulate PDF files and get detailed information about text and images in them. Features Quite A Box Of Tricks e. Quite A Box Of Tricks is a plug-in for Adobes Acrobat from Quite Software. The aim of Quite A Box Of Tricks is to make easy commonly needed PDF functions that were difficult, time consuming, expensive, or just impossible.

b28dd

manicapital.com-ErES full version
El Matador x manicapital.com
InstaCode Crack Win Serial Key
LA MANCHA DE ROLANDO - DISCOGRAFIA COMPLETA
mini magic box elite software download
Delcam PowerMill 10 SP6 .torrent
Wondershare MobileGo Multilingual Crack.
[RPG].[]manicapital.comb
sony vegas pro 13 crack khg team crack
Lazesoft Recover My Password Unlimited Edition Serials utorrent

Источник: [manicapital.com]
.

What’s New in the Quite A Box of Tricks v1.8-SHOCK serial key or number?

Screen Shot

System Requirements for Quite A Box of Tricks v1.8-SHOCK serial key or number

Add a Comment

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *