Total Recall 4.0 serial key or number

Total Recall 4.0 serial key or number

Total Recall 4.0 serial key or number

Total Recall 4.0 serial key or number

Killer Mobile Total Recall 4.20 Crack ->->->-> http://cinurl.com/12cuht
































































Total Recall Mobile Call Recorder 4.02 Download fresh windows warez idm adobe avast crack keygen nero facebookDownload total recall 4 20 . killer mobile total recall keygen, total recall cracked for s60v3,. 16 Nov 2014 Download total recall v.5.2.4 crack .To create more accurate search results for Killer Mobile Total Recall 3.0.1 try to exclude using commonly used keywords such as: crack, download, serial, keygen .Download Call Recorder Galaxy S8 apk old . is 4/5 from total 0 user votes. Call Recorder Galaxy S8 can be downloaded . the developer Killer Mobile all .. out of 1.43 Thousand at KeyOptimize.com. Total recall 4.20 keygen found at . killer-mobile-total-recall-4.20-keygen . for-total-recall-incl-crack.spet .Aggregate Recall Call Recorder by Killer Mobile . Call Recorder Total Recall FULL v2.0.33 download apk All Free .Total Recall Call Recorder is the most popular Call Recording with No Beep feature developed by Killer Mobile Software. It record your phone call .AquaSoft SlideShow 10 Ultimate v10 Total recall call recorder key generator. 5. 02 . Serial Total recall call recorder V 3.0.1 Crack Patch Keygen - k-cheats.Download Free Call Recorder APK Mirror . . crack, unlimited gold, gems . S6 & S7 Call Recorder Total Recall by Killer Mobile Total Recall is .Plus Total Recall is backed by Killer Mobile a US based developer that’s been around since 2003 and contains absolutely no privacy killing spam ads, or .Download Call Recorder Galaxy S8 apk old . is 4/5 from total 0 user votes. Call Recorder Galaxy S8 can be downloaded . the developer Killer Mobile all .Total Recall offers beep suppression . Total Recall Call Recorder (Symbian) for Mobile $19. . From Killer Mobile Software: Total Recall offers beep suppression .200 serials totalrecall 4 Keygen for total recall 4 03 keygen. 03 . killer mobile total recall v5 keygen . Total recall 4.20 registration code .200 serials totalrecall 4 Keygen for total recall 4 03 keygen. 03 . killer mobile total recall v5 keygen . Total recall 4.20 registration code .Torrentz will always love you Killer mobile total recall v4 keygen. . Request a Crack or Dongle Emulator . Download total recall 4 20 keygen .Mobile Total Recall 3. 03 Key Crack Full . TOTAL RECALL 4. 20 KEYGEN Killer Mobile . keygen , rapidshare, torrent Download total recall with .Download Total Recall 4. 20 Total Recall 4. 20 Crack Full Download Serial Keygen Many downloads like Total Recall 4. 20 may also include a cd key , serial .The Walgreen Company (simply Walgreens, or sometimes archaically Walgreen) is an American company that operates as the second-largest pharmacy store chain in the United States behind CVS. (killer,,,mobile,,,total,,,recall . recall serial number check crack windows 7 free download reflexive arcade keygen 2013 corvette kill switch pc game crack .To create more accurate search results for Total Recall Killer Mobile 4.3 try to exclude using commonly used keywords such as: crack, download, serial, keygen .total recall keygen s60v3. . Killer mobile from 0. Total 1 SymbianOS9 S60v3 Com X Keygen recall recall one. . sg lock dongle crack. 0.Windows Mobile Call Recorder . 114) Killer Mobile Total Call Recorder 2 (51) Call Recorder For Mobile Phones (169) Total Recall Mobile Call Recorder 4.20 Crack .total recall keygen s60v3. . Killer mobile from 0. Total 1 SymbianOS9 S60v3 Com X Keygen recall recall one. . sg lock dongle crack. 0.. crack Total Recall v5.1, . Keygen for total recall 5 1. . killer mobile total recall. txtmate m Sms like android sms in s60v2 operamini for smart .There are crack groups who work hard in order to unlock software, games, etc. If you search for Killer Mobile Total Recall 4.0 Crack, .Killer Mobile Totalrecall Serial Numbers. Convert Killer Mobile Totalrecall trail version to full software.Found 6 results for Killer Mobile Total Recall 3.0.1.All Killer Mobile Software applications come with a free, . Total Recall S60 Call Recorder 3.1.0 Misc. Internet software developed by Killer Mobile . crack .. crack Total Recall v5.1, . Keygen for total recall 5 1. . killer mobile total recall. txtmate m Sms like android sms in s60v2 operamini for smart .Free total recall mobile call recorder 4 20 . Call Recorder By Killer Mobile Software . Using Total Recall Mobile Call Recorder 4 20 Free Download crack, .The Walgreen Company (simply Walgreens, or sometimes archaically Walgreen) is an American company that operates as the second-largest pharmacy store chain in the United States behind CVSTotal Recall Call Recorder is the most trusted & reliable call recorder for Android mobile phones available - and best of all it’s FREE. . Killer Mobile.Total Recall Call Recorder - Total Recall by Killer Mobile is the best selling, most feature packed Symbian / S60 Call Recorder mobile .Galaxy S8 Call Recorder Total Recall Total Recall is the Best Selling CALL RECORDER for Samsung Galaxy devices including the S8, S8 Plus, S8 Edge, S7, S7 Edge & S6 .total recall nokia c5. total recall 4 20 reg code. . total security full crack zip free. total recall . total recall killer mobile v 5 and keygen for c6. total .treasures of montezuma 5 crack killer mobile total recall 4.20 keygen solucionario calculo conceptos y contextos james stewart 3 edicion.rar Sap2000 v15 crackPHONEKY - Free Killer Mobile Total Recall v4.20 Symbian Symbian App, download app to your mobile. 7984cf4209
http://doorktingridilar.blogcu.com/harvard-medical-school-registrar-transcript-request-afinador-photo-arboles-tradicional-sassy/34770752http://dayviews.com/ldatwitsi/523940379/http://dragalagded.rebelmouse.com/requisitos-para-entrar-a-ecuador-2011-20stock-fisico-with-puppets-local-direct-jobs-2518716753.htmlhttp://ropbocopelij.exblog.jp/27879905/https://fictionpad.com/author/lirecen/blog/404686/Pokemon-Black-Rom-English-Download-Free-Zip-spieledownload-partnervhttp://smalamun.yolasite.com/resources/descarga-musica-gratis-online-sin-registrarse.pdfhttp://dayviews.com/pucmepen/523940378/https://www.gitbook.com/book/bininipes/char-broil-tru-infrared-grill-review-vitaa-diam-s/detailshttps://arbotere.wixsite.com/cachafcode/single-post/2017/12/19/The-Game-Dope-Boys-Ft-Travis-Barker-Zippyhttp://syncroz-gaming.xooit.fr/viewtopic.php?p=561
 
Источник: [https://torrent-igruha.org/3551-portal.html]
, Total Recall 4.0 serial key or number

buzzrabono

Download

14 Jun 2014 bit.ly1jqDtUi Serial Total Recall v5.1, Serial Key Total Recall v5.1, crack Total Recall v5.1, Keygen Total Recall v5.1, Total Recall v5.1. 14 Jun 2014 bit.ly1jpR8eb Serial Total recall call recorder V 3.0.1, Serial Key Total recall call recorder V 3.0.1, crack Total recall call recorder V 3.0.1,. 22 Oct 2013 Total Recall is the easiest, most user friendly Call Recorder Mobile Comments (1) Keywords: total recall v5 0 keygen signed cracked, killer mobile total recall. txtmate m Sms like android sms in s60v2 operamini for smart how to use sisboom s60v3 om 7 5 globe apk 2015 chess symbian s60. By Antonio Wells Oct 17, 2013 5:20 PM – 13 Comments Call Recorder Total Recall 2 is the latest version of the many variations of Killer Mobile’s Cricket, Metro S3); i9100 (Samsung Galaxy S2 with OS 4.0.3 to 4.1.1); T989 (T-mobile Sprint.

.aac.au.avi.ca.cn.de.doc.es.eu.flac.fr.gb.gzip.id.in.iso.jar.jpeg.movie.mp3.mpeg.mpeg4.pdf.rar.swf.us.wma.zip15crackedforkeygenrecallSydneytotal

Download

Alco-Sensor IV Total Recall®is a Windows® application designed for (20-0300 -00) An available USB or 9-pin serial port is required for connecting your PC to the which lead the operator through the instrument result download process. 17 Oct 2013 Call Recorder Total Recall 2 is the latest version of the many variations of Killer Manual tweaking to figure out which recording method works for you.. By Pam Pamm on November 1, 2013 at 11:20 pm: Log in and app was downloaded but I can’t go back and get my old messages that was saved. 22 Oct 2013 mistermobile-KillerMobile Total Recall for Nokia Symbian code free download, killer mobile total recall keygen, total recall cracked for s60v3,. 16 Nov 2014 Download total recall v.5.2.4 crack direct download link (click and install) —> http: po.stK2p4gc or —> bit.ly1xdS9Qw ————— Extra. Total Recall Owner’s Manual — V2.20u PDF Remote Recall is client software for Total Recall. Calls and associated call. The call data (but not the voice files) will be downloaded via the serial link from Total Recall to the PC. Test Window. Total recall key free download (Symbian) – Total Recall 4.2.0: Utility for recording phone calls in AMR or Five Apps to Try This Weekend – February 20th 2015. 21 Sep 2010 Total Recall Call Recorder is the best call recording software for Android Mobile Phones. Automatically Record Calls, Voice Notes, Meetings,. 14 Jun 2014 bit.ly1jqDtUi Serial Total Recall v5.1, Serial Key Total Recall v5.1, crack Total Recall v5.1, Keygen Total Download it from Adobe. The series was inspired by the 1990 film Total Recall, based on Philip K. Dick’s short Rekall also provides the Operating Systems for the Androids manufactured by Uber Braun. “Paranoid”, April 20, 1999, Farve and Hume investigate the murder of the head of the. Create a book Download as PDF Printable version. Suade4life use magnet link the idea worked 4 me Donrrrt worry, i got the Excel Vba Crack Workbook Protection Litecam Download Crack Crack The Sims 3 Seasons Only Poweramp Cracked Apk Root Total Recall 5 Keygen Download Please Oughout make waste materials my 20 days within downloading & 72 hours.

.aac.au.avi.ca.cn.de.doc.es.eu.flac.fr.gb.gzip.id.in.iso.jar.jpeg.movie.mp3.mpeg.mpeg4.pdf.rar.swf.us.wma.zip204crackeddownloadkeygenrecallSwanseatotal

Download

22 Oct 2013 Total Recall is the easiest, most user friendly Call Recorder Mobile Dictaphone for Here are some key features of “Total Recall (S60): mobile total recall keygen, total recall cracked for s60v3, total recall call recorder. Total Recall S60 Call Recorder – Total Recall ™ 3.1.0 Call Recorder by Killer but the only problem it does’nt has keygen and some time it does’nt send the. Symbian Call Recorder App | Beep Free Call Recorder for Nokia S60 Devices. Total Recall is the easiest, most user friendly Call Recorder Mobile Dictaphone for Symbian S60 devices. Total Recall by Killer Mobile, feature packed Call. 15 Dec 2008 Total Recall S60 Call Recorder free download. Get the latest version now. Quite simply the most User Friendly Call Recorder Utility for S60. 12 Jan 2013 Download Total Recall (Symbian) 5.5.2 Free in Telephony. Total Recall ( Symbian) – Call Recorder Utility for S60 Devices. Please be aware that Brothersoft do not supply any crack, patches, serial numbers or keygen for Total Recall Call Recorder for S60 3rd,and please consult . 10 Nov 2008 Please be aware that Brothersoft do not supply any crack, patches, serial numbers or keygen for Total Recall Call Recorder for S60 3rd,and. 15 May 2013 Total Recall offers beep suppression available to deliver high quality, beep free call recording on literally ALL Symbian^3 and Nokia S60. 1 Dec 2009 Total Recall by Killer Mobile is the best selling, feature packed Call Recorder utility for Nokia S60 devices running the Symbian OS.

.aac.au.avi.ca.cn.de.doc.es.eu.flac.fr.gb.gzip.id.in.iso.jar.jpeg.movie.mp3.mpeg.mpeg4.pdf.rar.swf.us.wma.zipcallcrackedforkeygenLiverpoolrecallrecorders60total
Источник: [https://torrent-igruha.org/3551-portal.html]
Total Recall 4.0 serial key or number

Intonation does aid serial recall after all

Abstract

A sequence of spoken digits is easier to recall if the digits are grouped into smaller chunks (e.g., through the insertion of pauses). It has been claimed that intonation does not facilitate recall over and above the effect achieved by pauses. This may be related to the fact that past research has used synthesized intonation contours. In this replication study, we show that intonation does provide benefits once more naturalistic intonation contours are used. This benefit is independent of response modality (spoken responses, keyboard responses, or handwritten responses in a grid). We furthermore show that intonation differentially affects specific positions within the sequence of digits. Crucially, our results suggest that researchers and clinicians need to pay attention to intonation when assessing working memory using spoken language.

Serial recall of digits is one of the major ways working memory capacity is assessed, both in research and in clinical settings (Baddeley, Eysenck, & Anderson, 2009; Conway et al., 2005; Wechsler, 1987). It is well established that recall is facilitated when digits are presented in chunks (Crowder & Greene, 2000), especially when presented acoustically (Cowan, Saults, & Brown, 2004; Frankish, 1985), in which the chunks are separated by pauses. What is still unclear is how far intonation—the melody of speech—further facilitates serial recall. According to Frankish (1995) and Saito (1998), intonation can aid recall only insofar as it is involved in chunking, with little effect over and above the effect of pause insertion. In our study, we directly compare the recall of spoken digit sequences grouped by pauses against sequences with specific intonation contours. Our results show that intonation does make a difference after all.

In his study, Frankish (1995) directly compared the effect of intonation and pausing on serial recall using synthesized speech. Pitch contours were obtained by instructing a speaker “to use a strongly-accented intonation pattern to group the sequences into threes” (p. 59); the resulting contours were superimposed onto the synthesized speech. In addition, there was an intonation condition with additional pauses grouping the list into threes, as well as a pause condition with monotone pitch. Frankish found that recall performance did not differ significantly across any of the three grouped conditions (intonation, intonation + pause, pause), which was taken to suggest that pitch movements do not contribute beyond a grouping effect that can be obtained by means of pauses alone.

We reasoned that factors could have hampered the effect of intonation in Frankish’s experiment: (1) The pitch contour may have been unnatural, as it was transplanted from one utterance to another, and (2) the speaker was specifically asked to chunk the list into groups of threes, whereas the intonational structure of lists can have a more complex organization (Hirschberg & Pierrehumbert, 1986; Tyler, 2014). There is cross-linguistic evidence that intonation cues hierarchical grouping in lists, with various rising contours signalling non-final (earlier in lists) and penultimate positions, and falling contours signalling final position (Bolinger, 1989; Geluykens & Swerts, 1994; Ladd, 1980, 2008; Pierrehumbert & Hirschberg, 1990; Savino, 2001, 2004; Savino, Grice, Gili Fivela, & Marotta, 2006; Swerts, Collier, & Terken, 1994). Moreover, when listening, participants were explicitly instructed to use the grouping-into-three strategy and were provided with grids for writing their answers that suggested this grouping. This way of delivering responses biases participants toward chunking, which could have further masked any differences between the different conditions.

In this study, we investigate whether the use of naturalistic intonation patterns conveying positional information facilitates serial recall beyond the grouping effect, especially in certain positions (end of group, end of sequence, penultimate in the group, penultimate in the sequence). To this aim, we replicated Frankish’s (1995) serial recall experiment using natural stimuli with pitch contours conveying positional information. In addition, we compared different recall tasks, including tasks that did not explicitly bias participants toward chunking.

Finally, some studies have reported that recall performance can be facilitated with written-on-grid responses, compared with spoken (Harvey & Beaman, 2007; Penney, 1979) or keyboard responses (Penney & Blackwood, 1989). However, these studies comparing recall modalities did not consider the effect of intonation. Thus, our experiment combines an intonational manipulation (two natural intonation contours, a pause condition, and a no-pause no-intonation control condition) with a response modality manipulation (written-on-grid, keyboard, spoken recall).

Method

Conditions

The study was conducted in Italian, specifically the variety spoken in Bari. As is the case with other languages, Bari Italian has a number of pitch contours for cueing positional information of items in sequences (Savino, 2001, 2004; Savino et al., 2006). Positions that are pre-final (penultimate in each triplet and in list) and non-final (any other position that is not final in list) are signalled by different types of rising contours, whereas final position is signalled by a fall (see Table 1 in the Appendix for details on all contour types). On the basis of this tonal inventory, two list types were compiled: Intonation Contour A and Intonation Contour B:

  • Intonation Contour A had an intonation contour at the end of the first and second triplets (Positions 3 and 6) signalling non-finality, and a final contour at the end of the entire list (Position 9).

  • Intonation Contour B additionally had a contour signalling pre-finality in each triplet, and in list (Positions 2, 5 and 8).

Two additional list types had a neutral falling contour on all digits. The Grouped-by-Pauses condition had a pause after Positions 3 and 6, whereas the Ungrouped condition had no pauses. For a schematic representation of these four prosodic patterns, see Fig. 3 in the Appendix.

Preparation of stimuli

To construct the stimuli, we first produced sequences of the same digit in all nine positions with Intonation Contour A, Intonation Contour B, and with the neutral falling contour. For example, for digit uno (one), the sequence “uno uno uno uno uno uno uno uno uno” was produced once with Contour A, once with Contour B, and once with a neutral falling contour on each digit. In this way, all intonational realizations for each position in each prosodic condition were available for each digit, taking into account downtrends in fundamental frequency (F0) across stretches of natural speech (Ladd, 1984). All sequences were produced by a trained speaker of Bari Italian (author M.S.) in the same recording session. All digit renditions were saved as individual audio files and were used as “building blocks” for creating the stimuli for all experimental conditions, by concatenating the individual audio files into nine-digit sequences.

Spoken digit renditions with the neutral falling pitch shape were concatenated to create sequence stimuli for the conditions Ungrouped (control) and Grouped-by-Pauses. In the latter case, a 310-ms long silence was inserted after digits in Positions 3 and 6. Digits produced with Intonation Contour A and Intonation Contour B were used for creating sequences of these two intonation contour types, respectively. An example of a digit sequence for each of the four prosodic conditions is shown in Fig. 1.

We produced 17 stimuli for each experimental condition, for a total amount of 68, including eight stimuli to be used in the training session (two per prosodic condition). The duration of each stimulus sequence averaged 6.4 s. The concatenated nine-digit sequences were created on the basis of 68 nine-digit lists we derived by pseudo-random permutation of the 1–9 digits, avoiding two adjacent digits in ascending or descending order, or the same digit in an identical position in consecutive lists. All steps for the preparation of stimuli were carried out using Praat (Boersma, 2001).

Participants

Seventy-eight participants (63 female, 15 male, Mage = 22.35 years, SD = 3.29 years) took part in the experiment for course credit. They were undergraduate and graduate students of psychology at the University of Bari, all born and living in the Bari dialectal area. Participants did not report any speech or hearing deficits, and they did not have any background in phonetics or speech science.

Procedure

Participants were tested individually in a quiet laboratory, sitting in front of a computer and wearing headphones. They were instructed to listen to each sequence and recall all nine digits in the same order in which they were presented (the importance of recalling in the correct order was emphasized in the instructions). Participants responded immediately after the presentation of the last digit. No grouping strategy was suggested.

Each list was preceded by a 890-ms tone (263 Hz), followed by 500 ms of silence. After each response, participants proceeded to the next sequence by pressing the space bar. They were allowed to pause whenever they wanted during the session, and they were encouraged to take a break after every block of 15 stimuli. Stimuli from the same condition were blocked, with block order balanced across participants. Before starting the task, participants were tested using the WAIS-R Digit Span test (Wechsler, 1987).

In contrast to the stimuli manipulation, which was within participants, the response modality manipulation was between participants. A group of 29 participants (23 female, six male, Mage = 22.8 years, SD = 4.55, digit span = 6.76, SD = 0.77) were asked to recall the lists orally. Participants in this condition wore a microphone for recording their responses. Another group of 24 participants (20 female, four male, Mage = 22.33, SD = 2.64, digit span = 6.5, SD = 0.96) were instructed to write down each sequence in a nine-box grid drawn on paper, from left to right (in contrast to Frankish, 1995, grids were not drawn in a way to overtly suggesting grouping into triplets). They were instructed to fill all nine boxes in the grid even if they were unsure of the correct response. A third group of 25 participants (20 female, five male, Mage = 21.84, SD = 1.46, digit span = 6.48, SD = 0.81) performed the task by typing the digits on a computer keyboard, and pressing the “return” key after the end of each recalled sequence. Each session (i.e., including the digit span test, and independently from the recall modality) lasted approximately 40 min. Trials were implemented and run using SuperLab 2.0 (Cedrus Corporation, 1991).

Statistical analysis

We used R (Version 3.6.0; R Core Team, 2019) and the package brms 2.9.0 (Bürkner, 2017) to compute a mixed Bayesian logistic regression model on the accuracy scores. The main fixed effects were response modality (spoken, keyboard, grid) and condition (Intonation A, Intonation B, pause, control). In addition, we included a fixed effect for “position within triplet,” which was added as a monotonic variable (see Bürkner & Charpentier, 2018). This variable codes for the first, second, and third position within each triplet (1, 2, 3 versus 4, 5, 6 versus 7, 8, 9). Thus, the first “position within triplet” codes for Positions 1, 4, and 7; the second codes for 2, 5, 8; and the third for 3, 6, 9.

As fixed effects, we also included a Position Within Triplet × Condition interaction, as well as a Response Modality × Condition interaction. Digit span and overall position (1 to 9) were added as control variables. To account for primacy and recency effects, we added overall position also as a squared predictor, which models the parabolic shape seen in most serial recall curves. The random effects component included random intercepts for participant as well as random slopes for all within-participant variables (including random slopes for interactions) and correlation terms between all random effect components. Markov Chain Monte Carlo sampling was performed with 4,000 iterations for four chains (2,000 warm-up), resulting in 8,000 posterior samples. There was no indication of any convergence issues (all Rhat = 1.0). Posterior predictive checks indicated no issues.

All data and code for the statistical analyses are made available under the following OSF repository: https://osf.io/5b94c

Results

Items were scored as correct only if they were recalled in the same serial position in which they were presented. Results of recall performance as a function of output modality show that keyboard responses (71.2%) were on average more accurate than grid responses (69.3%), which were in turn better than spoken responses (63.7%; see Fig. 2a). However, model comparisons (10-fold cross-validation) show that the model with the modality main effect did not lead to better predictions than the model without this effect (k-fold IC difference: −4.14, SE = 26.98). This suggests that, overall, there are no stark average differences between response modalities in this study.Footnote 1

Overall performance was similar for Intonation Contour A (74.7%) and Intonation Contour B (72.9%), which were both higher in accuracy than the pause condition (68.8%) and the control condition (54.8%; see Fig. 2b). Model comparison with 10-fold cross-validation showed that the model with the condition main effect reliably improved predictive performance (k-fold IC difference: 111.35, SE = 33.95). In terms of parameter estimates, compared to the control condition (reference level), all three conditions are associated with positive coefficients (higher accuracy) whose 95% Bayesian credible intervals are far from zero: Contour A \( \left(\hat{\beta}=1.23, SE=0.11,95\% CI:\left[1.02,1.44\right]\right) \), Contour B \( \left(\hat{\beta}=1.06, SE=0.10,\left[0.87,1.25\right]\right) \), and pause \( \left(\hat{\beta}=0.85, SE=0.03,\left[0.49,0.61\right]\right) \).Footnote 2 Comparison of the posterior samples for the respective coefficients shows that there is a high posterior probability for contour A (\( {\hat{\beta}}_A>{\hat{\beta}}_{pause}=1.0 \)) and contour B (\( {\hat{\beta}}_B>{\hat{\beta}}_{pause}=0.97 \)) being overall more accurate than the pause condition. The posterior probability for Contour A being more accurate than B was also high (\( {\hat{\beta}}_A>{\hat{\beta}}_B=0.98 \)).

Figure 2c shows the serial recall accuracy curves with the familiar U shape that is generated by primacy and recency effects. The descriptive statistics based on the raw values show that for the first position the average accuracy was quite similar for all non-control conditions (Contour A: 69.8%, Contour B: 67.6%, pause: 67%) when broken up by position within a triplet. However, for the third (final) position within a triplet, both intonation contours, A (83.8%) and B (83.9%), led to much better performances than the pause condition (76.0%). The second (penultimate) position also showed some differences between Intonation Contours A (70.6%) and B (67.2%) compared with the pause condition (63.4%); however, these were not as pronounced. On the other hand, the average accuracy was worse in Contour B than in Contour A for this position, contrary to our expectation. Since in Contour B the penultimate position in a triplet is intonationally marked as such, we expected listeners to make use of that intonational cue, resulting in higher accuracy in recalling that position in B than in A.

Figure 2d shows the marginal posterior predictions of the logistic regression model for position within triplet, broken up by condition. This clearly shows that there was an advantage for the intonation conditions, over and above pause, specifically for the third position within each triplet.

Crucially, the model with the Condition × Position Within Triplet interaction term performed reliably better in terms of predictive accuracy than the model without this term (k-fold IC difference: 10946.3, SE = 107.8). In terms of parameter estimates, Contours A and B, as well as the pause condition, had interaction coefficients that were far from zero. While the pause condition was associated with a slight boost for later positions within each triplet \( \left(\hat{\beta}=0.37, SE=0.05,\left[0.28,0.46\right]\right) \), this boost was much stronger for both contour A \( \left(\hat{\beta}=0.60, SE=0.06,\left[0.48,0.72\right]\right) \) and contour B \( \left(\hat{\beta}=0.63, SE=0.05,\left[0.52,0.73\right]\right) \). Thus, the intonation contours led to increased accuracy, specifically for later positions within each triplet (see Fig. 2d). A direct comparison of the posterior samples for these interaction coefficients shows that the posterior probability of the interaction with position within triplet was stronger for Contour A than for the pause condition (\( {\hat{\beta}}_A>{\hat{\beta}}_{pause}=0.99 \)), and the same was the case for comparing Contour B to the pause condition (\( {\hat{\beta}}_B>{\hat{\beta}}_{pause}=1.0 \)). On the other hand, there was little evidence for Contours A and B differing from each other in terms of this interaction effect (\( {\hat{\beta}}_A>{\hat{\beta}}_B=0.33 \)). This suggests that both intonation contours receive a similar boost for the position within triplet.

While the position within triplet factor reliably interacted with condition, there was no strong indication for an interaction between response modality and condition. Cross-validation showed that the model with and without this interaction term did not differ reliably in predictive performance (k-fold IC = 6.25, SE = 24.53).

Discussion and conclusions

Our results show that when using naturalistic intonation contours, intonation facilitates serial recall as compared with simple pause grouping, especially for items in specific positions: end of first and second triplets, and end of the whole sequence. This indicates that intonation provides an extra cue to chunking, suggesting that a rising intonation marking the end of non-final triplets is perceptually more salient than a pause, highlighting the final digit in the triplet. Similarly, it suggests that a falling intonation at the end of a whole sequence is a clearer signal to finality than a pause.

Our results differ from those obtained by Frankish (1995), possibly for two reasons: (1) In our stimuli, we use a natural (pre-recorded) voice instead of synthesized speech, and (2) unlike English, Italian words for digits are mostly disyllabic, allowing for more time for the intonation contour to unfold, making them better able to lend salience to the digit if a rise is used. Future research needs to replicate this result for other languages to decide between these two explanations; however, regardless of which of these two accounts most likely explains our results, we have shown that intonation does matter after all. Although our results suggest that having many different intonational cues is associated with diminishing returns, as there was no strong difference between Contours A and B, the beneficial effect of intonation in serial recall was strong enough to be consistently observed across all response modalities.

Our findings have implications for methodology in digit span assessment. Current protocol prescribes that clinicians read aloud digit sequences to be recalled by using a “monotone intonation.” Since this is a difficult task, even for trained phoneticians, clinicians are very likely to inadvertently introduce some intonational marking that will differ from one clinician to another. For instance, if an individual is tested at two time points but with a different clinician, a reduction in digit span might be a function of the clinicians’ own intonation patterns rather than a difference in the subject’s working memory capacity. Pre-recorded standardized materials, along the lines of those used in this study (see also Norris, Hall, & Gathercole, 2019, for using a similar methodology in a digit span task), would aid comparability across individuals and sessions. This would be crucial, for example, in longitudinal clinical studies involving digit span assessment.

This paper contributes to our understanding of how human listeners use cues to structure when constructing (and reporting) memory traces. Future work will hopefully contribute toward determining the effects of digit span of stimuli with no pauses, as sometimes occurs in naturally spoken utterances. We hope to inspire further studies on other languages and dialects and how they signal the substructure of grouped lists, and those cue patterns influence digit span.

Notes

  1. 1.

    The small differences in performance between response modalities may be related to their intrinsic differences in response time (keyboard responses faster than written on grid, the latter faster than spoken responses due to hesitations or disfluencies). We did not record response time in our study; however, that a different delay in memory recovery (as a consequence of different response time) might influence the recovery process and therefore recall accuracy is a plausible explanation, which is worth verifying in a future study. Thanks to D. Robert Ladd for having brought this issue to our attention.

  2. 2.

    To facilitate the interpretation of these effects, we report coefficients from the model without interactions.

References

  1. Baddeley, A., Eysenck M.W., & Anderson, M. C. (2009). Memory. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar

  2. Boersma, P. (2001). Praat, a system for doing phonetics by computer. Glot International, 5(9/10), 131-151.

    Google Scholar

  3. Bolinger, D. (1989). Intonation and its uses: Melody in grammar and discourse. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar

  4. Bürkner, P.-C. (2017). brms: An R Package for Bayesian multilevel models using Stan. Journal of Statistical Software, 80(1), 1–28. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v080.i01

    Article Google Scholar

  5. Bürkner, P. C., & Charpentier, E. (2018). Monotonic effects: A principled approach for including ordinal predictors in regression models. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/9qkhj

  6. Cedrus Corporation. (1991). SuperLab 2.0 [Computer software]. San Pedro, CA: Author. Retrieved from https://cedrus.com/superlab/index.htm

    Google Scholar

  7. Conway, A. R. A., Kane, M. J., Bunting, M. F., Hambrick, D. Z., Wilhelm, O., & Eagle, R. W. (2005). Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s guide. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(5), 769-786. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196772

    Article Google Scholar

  8. Cowan, N., Saults, J. S., & Brown, G. D. A. (2004). On the auditory modality superiority effect in serial recall: Separating input and output factors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30(3), 639-644. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.30.3.639

    ArticlePubMed Google Scholar

  9. Crowder, R. G., & Greene, R. L. (2000). Serial learning: Cognition and behaviour. In E. Tulving & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 125–136). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar

  10. Frankish, C. (1985). Modality-specific grouping effect in short-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 24(2), 200-209. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(85)90024-5

    Article Google Scholar

  11. Frankish, C. (1995). Intonation and auditory grouping in immediate serial recall. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 9(7), 5-22. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.2350090703

    Article Google Scholar

  12. Geluykens, R., & Swerts, M. (1994). Prosodic cues to discourse boundaries in experimental dialogues. Speech Communication, 15(1/2), 69-77. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-6393(94)90042-6

    Article Google Scholar

  13. Harvey, A. J., & Beaman, C. P. (2007). Input and output effects in immediate serial recall. Memory, 15(7), 693-700. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210701644677

    ArticlePubMed Google Scholar

  14. Hirschberg, J., & Pierrehumbert, J. (1986). The intonational structuring of discourse. Proceedings of the 24th Annual Meeting of the Association of Computational Linguistics (pp. 136-144). https://doi.org/10.3115/981131.981152

  15. Ladd, D. R. (1980). The structure of intonational meaning: Evidence from English. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar

  16. Ladd, D. R. (1984). Declination: A review and some hypotheses. Phonology, 1, 53-74. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952675700000294

    Article Google Scholar

  17. Ladd, D. R. (2008). Intonational phonology (Cambridge Studies in Linguistics). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar

  18. Norris, D., Hall, J., & Gathercole, S. (2019). Can short-term memory be trained? Memory & Cognition, 47(5), 1012-1023. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-019-00901-z

    Article Google Scholar

  19. Penney, C. (1979). Interactions of suffix effects with suffix delay and recall modality in serial recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5(5), 507-521. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.5.5.507

    Article Google Scholar

  20. Penney, C., & Blackwood P. A. (1989). Recall mode and recency in immediate serial recall: computer users beware! Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 27(6), 545-547. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03334664

    Article Google Scholar

  21. Pierrehumbert, J., & Hirschberg, J. (1990). The meaning of intonation in the interpretation of discourse. In P. Cohen, J. Morgan, & M. Pollack (Eds.), Intentions in communication (pp. 271-311). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar

  22. R Core Team. (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Computer software]. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/

    Google Scholar

  23. Saito, S. (1998). Effects of articulatory suppression on immediate serial recall of temporarily grouped and intonated lists. Psychologia, 41(2), 95-101.

    Google Scholar

  24. Savino, M. (2001). Non-finality and pre-finality in Bari Italian intonation: A preliminary account. Proceedings of the 7th European Conference on Speech Communication and Technology (pp. 939-942).https://www.isca-speech.org/archive/eurospeech_2001/e01_0943.html

  25. Savino, M. (2004). Intonational cues to discourse structure in a variety of Italian. In P. Gilles and J. Peters (Eds.), Regional variation in intonation (161-187). Tuebingen, Germany: Niemeyer.

    Google Scholar

  26. Savino M., Grice, M., Gili Fivela, B. & Marotta, G. (2006). Intonational cues to discourse structure in Bari and Pisa Italian: Perceptual evidence. Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2006 [CD-ROM].

  27. Swerts, M., Collier, R., Terken, J. (1994). Prosodic predictors of discourse finality in spontaneous monologues. Speech Communication, 15(1/2), 79-90. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-6393(94)90043-4

    Article Google Scholar

  28. Tyler, J. (2014). Rising pitch, continuation, and the hierarchical structure of discourse. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics, 20(1), 36. Retrieved from https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol20/iss1/36

    Google Scholar

  29. Wechsler, D. (1987). Manual for WAIS-R. New York, NY: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar

Download references

Author information

Affiliations

  1. Department of Education, Psychology, Communication, University of Bari, Bari, Italy

    Michelina Savino & Andrea Bosco

  2. Department of English Language & Linguistics, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, UK

    Bodo Winter

  3. IfL-Phonetics, University of Cologne, Cologne, Germany

    Martine Grice

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michelina Savino.

Источник: [https://torrent-igruha.org/3551-portal.html]
.

What’s New in the Total Recall 4.0 serial key or number?

Screen Shot

System Requirements for Total Recall 4.0 serial key or number

Add a Comment

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *